Effects of music on the spatial reasoning skills of grade-one pupils
Abstract
The study determined the significant effect of different types of music on the spatial reasoning skills of children aging from 6-8 years old. 21 grade one students (13 males, 8 females) of Colegio de San Juan de Letran were selected to complete a puzzle-solving task to assess their spatial reasoning skills under the two different conditions based on the independent variable of music. These two types of independent variable were Instrumental Music (Binaural Beats) and Nursery Rhymes (Old McDonald). The results were all calculated using Wilcoxon’s Matched-Pairs Signed Rank Test. Findings show that nursery rhymes do stimulate the brain in processing images more accurately than that of instrumental music. Children ranging from 6-8 years old are more likely to perform better on a spatial reasoning task if they are listening to fast-beat nursery rhymes, than that of instrumental music. Limitations and suggestions for further studies were discussed.
Keywords
Full Text:
PDFReferences
Alley, T. & Greene, M. (2008). The Relative and Perceived Impact of Irrelevant Speech, Vocal Music and Non-vocal Music on Working Memory. Current Psychology 27, 277–289
Berz, W.L. (1995). Working Memory in Music: A Theoretical Model. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12(3), 353-364
Bower, B. (1994). Tuning up young brains. Retrieved August 9, 2013 from http://www.thefreelibrary.com/Tuning+up+young+brains.-a015811444
Boyle, G. (1983). Effects on academic learning of manipulating emotional states and motivational dynamics. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 53, 347-357.
Chaplin, J.P. (1985). In Dictionary of Psychology. Canada: Bantam Dell, Random House, Inc.
El Haj, M., Postal, V., & Allain, P. (2012). Music Enhances Autobiographical Memory in Mild Alzheimer’s Disease. Educational Gerontology, 38, 30–41.
Furnham, A. & Bradley, A. (1997). Music While You Work: The Differential Distraction of Background Music on the Cognitive Test Performance of Introverts and Extraverts. Applied Cognitive Psychology 11, 445-455
Fraser, C., & Bradford, J. A. (2013). Music to Your Brain: Background Music Changes Are Processed First , Reducing Ad Message Recall. Psychology and Marketing. 62–75.
Goodin, P., Ciorciari, J., Baker, K., Carey, A.-M., Carrey, A.-M., Harper, M., & Kaufman, J. (2012). A high-density EEG investigation into steady state binaural beat stimulation. PloS one, 7(4), 347-389.
Hallam, S. (2008). The Powerful Role of Music in Society. Retrieved July 30, 2013 from http://musicmagic.wordpress.com/2008/07/10/music-in-society/
Hallam, S., Price, J., & Katsarou, G., (2002). The Effects of Background Music on Primary School Pupils' Task Performance, Educational Studies, 28(2).
Hammond, C. (2013). Does listening to Mozart really boost your brainpower? Retrieved August 9, 2013 from http://www.bbc.com/future/story/20130107-can-mozart-boost-brainpower/1
Hergenhahn, B. & Olson, M. (2007). Introduction to Theories of Learning. (7th ed). New Jersey: Pearson
Hjortsberg, R. (2009). The effects of different types of music on cognitive processes. Retrieved July 20, 2013 from http://clearinghouse.missouriwestern.edu/manuscripts/303.php
Husain, G., Thompson, W., & Schellenberg, G. (2002). Effects of Musical Tempo and Mode on Arousal, Mood, and Spatial Abilities. Music Perception, 20, 151-171.
Isen, A., Daubmann, K., & Nowicki, G. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122-1131.
Jenkins, J.S. (2001). The Mozart effect. Journal of The Royal Society Of Medicine 94, 170-172
Kaplan, R.M., & Saccuzzo, D. P. (2009). Standardized tests in education, civil service, and the military. Psychological testing: Principles, applications, and issues (7 ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Kenney, S. (2005). Nursery Rhymes: Foundation for Learning. General Music Today, 19(1), 28-31.
Kokotsaki, D. & Hallam, S. (2011) The perceived benefits of participative music making for non-music university students: a comparison with music students. Music Education Research, 13, 149-172.
Lachance, J. A, & Mazzocco, M. M. M. (2006). A longitudinal analysis of sex differences in math and spatial skills in primary school age children. Learning and individual differences, 16(3), 195–216.
Levine, S.C., Ratliff K.R., Huttenlocher, J., and Cannon, J. (2012). Early puzzle play: A predictor of preschoolers' spatial transformation skill. Developmental Psychology, 48, 530-542.
Mayfield, C and Moss, S (1989) Effect of music tempo on task performance. Psychological Reports: Volume. 65, 1283-1290.
McLeod, S. (2013). Erik Erikson. Retrieved August 9, 2013 from http://www.simplypsychology.org/Erik-Erikson.html
Nantais, K. & Schellenberg, G. (1999). The Mozart Effect: An Artifact of Preference. Psychological Science, 10, 370-373
O’Hanlon, J. (1981). Boredom: Practical consequences and a theory. Acta Psychologica, 49, 53-82.
Olsen, K. (1996). The Effects of Music on the Mind: Beyond Soothing the Savage Beast. Retrieved August 9, 2013 from http://www.reversespins.com/effectsofmusic.html
Pietschnig, J., Voracek, M., & Formann, A. (2010). Mozart Effect-Shmozart Effect: A Meta-Analysis. Intelligence, 38, 314-323.
Perham, N. & Vizard, J. (2011). Can Preference for Background Music Mediate the Irrelevant Sound Effect? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 625–631
Pring, L. & Walker, J. (1994). The Effects of Unvocalized Music on Short-Term Memory. Current Psychology 13, 165-17
Salame, P., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Effects of background music on phonological short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 107–122.
Schellenberg, E.G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Science, 15, 511–514.
Thompson, W.F., Schellenberg, E.G., & Letnic, A.K. (2012). Fast and loud background music hinders reading comprehension. Psychology of Music, 40, 700-708.
Refbacks
- There are currently no refbacks.
e-ISSN: 1694-2116
p-ISSN: 1694-2493